Research Article |
Corresponding author: Barbora Ďurajková ( barboradurajkova@gmail.com ) Academic editor: Ivanklin Campos-Filho
© 2022 Barbora Ďurajková, Richard Hladký, Ivan Hadrián Tuf.
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Citation:
Ďurajková B, Hladký R, Tuf IH (2022) Higher temperature and substrate vibrations as stress factors for terrestrial isopods – model species matter. In: De Smedt P, Taiti S, Sfenthourakis S, Campos-Filho IS (Eds) Facets of terrestrial isopod biology. ZooKeys 1101: 71-85. https://doi.org/10.3897/zookeys.1101.77549
|
This study was focused on behaviour of the Common Rough Woodlouse (Porcellio scaber) and the Plum Woodlouse (Porcellionides pruinosus) under the influence of stressors in the form of increased temperature, the vibrating surface, or their combination. Two types of experiments were performed. First, woodlice placed in a labyrinth were observed, to determine the degree of turn alternation, the speed of passing through the labyrinth, and the corrections of turn alternation, when exposed to stressors. In the second experiment how woodlice aggregate in the aforementioned potential stressors was recorded and whether the change in aggregation behaviour can be an indicator of the degree of stress. Increased temperature and the combination of increased temperature and vibrations were stressors only for P. scaber. The results show that vibrations are not a stress factor for P. scaber or P. pruinosus. Porcellio scaber passed through the labyrinth more slowly at increased temperatures, and although they made more turn-corrections, they alternated turns less intensely. Its aggregation behaviour was mainly influenced by temperature, which confirms that the aggregation behaviour of P. scaber actually indicates a degree of stress.
Aggregation, Isopoda, Oniscidea, stress factor, turn alternation
Humans are not the only ones to face stress. Although this may seem trivial from today’s perspective, soil invertebrates such as isopods may also suffer from stress (
In the present study, we examined two types of defence behaviour of isopods, namely systematic turn alternation and the formation of an aggregation. A tendency to alternate turns is a behaviour known for different organisms including humans. Turn alternations are characterised by two types of reactions. A spontaneous reaction (
The formation of aggregations can be considered as an evolutionary successful reaction to unfavourable temperature, water loss, or predator pressure (
Despite turn alternations,
Porcellio scaber Latreille, 1804 (9–14 mm length) were hand-picked in an urban area of the village Bučovice, while Porcellionides pruinosus (Brandt, 1833) (3–8 mm length) were collected from a garden compost in the town of Hodonín. Woodlice were placed in 17 × 17 × 8 cm plastic boxes with a thin layer of plaster to maintain humidity, with egg cartons used as an underlay. The plaster was kept moist, and isopods were fed on carrots ad libitum. Animals were kept in constant darkness with a temperature of 18–20 °C.
The behavioural reaction to two stress factors or their combination was observed in both species. The first factor was increased temperature (27–31 °C, treatment coded as T+) while normal temperature (18–24 °C, called lower temperature and coded at T-) was used as a control. The second factor was the presence of microvibrations (coded as V+) and the absence of vibrations served as a control (V-). The experiments were carried out from August to October 2020. Before the beginning of each experiment, woodlice were exposed to a specific combination of conditions (T+V+, T+V-, T-V+, or T-V-, respectively) for two hours.
Two speakers with a power of 5 W were used to test the responses of the isopods to non-specific vibrations. There was a chipboard plate on the top of the speakers. The vibrations were produced by an artificially created recording, the same as the recording used by
To measure alternating turn behaviour, we used a plastic T-maze of size 15 × 11 cm consisting of two parts, i.e., the bottom labyrinth part and the cover with a small hole that served as an entry spot for subjects. Isopods were placed into the maze with entomological forceps, and then we observed how they alternate turns when trying to reach one of the six possible ending points. To prevent that woodlouse follow conspecific cues left by the previous woodlouse, the plastic labyrinth was placed on white A4 paper which was replaced after each trial. We also recorded changes in isopod´s turn decision (isopod made U-turn and used opposite corridor), and the time needed for reaching one of the end points of the labyrinth (Fig.
To observe aggregation behaviour, 30 individuals of the same species were placed into a box, and recorded on camera for two hours. Before the experiment, the plaster inside each box was thoroughly moistened to provide enough humidity. For filming, a small Niceboy outdoor camera installed on a tripod was used. We analysed 12 images (one every 10 min) of each video and calculated the number of isopods touching each other, i.e., the presence of thigmotaxis. After filming, isopods were returned to the breeding boxes. The results are expressed as the average aggregation dynamics for all four variants of observation. In total, 46 aggregation dynamics of P. scaber and 49 aggregation dynamics of P. pruinosus were analysed.
For T-maze experiments, we analysed the level of turn alternations according to the end point, the time needed for reaching the end point, and the number of changes in turn alternation. For aggregation experiments, the number of aggregated animals (individuals in contact) every ten min were analysed. All results were evaluated using a one-way ANOVA with a significance level of α = 0.05. The presence of vibrations with the increased temperature was coded by the number 1, while the absence of both factors was marked as 0. Pearson’s correlation test was used to evaluate the dependence of the turn alternation and the speed of passage through a maze.
Three behavioural characteristics of movements were examined in the maze. The first was the rate of a random ramble (negatively correlated with turn alternation). The second variable was the time spent in the labyrinth, measured from the entry of an isopod into the maze until it reached one of the possible end points. The third variable was the extent of changes in turn alternation pattern, i.e., the number of returns and changes in the turn alternation in the labyrinth. A total of 280 individuals of P. scaber and 301 individuals of P. pruinosus were tested in this type of experiment.
Our results did not show a statistically significant association between the rate of a random/unspecific ramble (reversed value of systematic turn alternation) and the presence of microvibrations (F = 0.09; p = 0.761) for P. scaber. The average rate of a random ramble for the presence of vibrations was 1.74 and for the absence of vibrations was 1.71. There was no significant effect of vibrations on the time spent in the labyrinth (F = 1.45; p = 0.229), although the individuals of P. scaber exposed to vibrations ran through the labyrinth with an average time of 38 sec vs. 45 sec with the absence of vibrations. Also, there was no significant association between the presence of vibrations and changing of the turn alternation pattern for this species (F = 0.20; p = 0.657). An average number of changes during the presence of vibrations was 0.87 in contrast with 0.77 during their absence.
In contrast, for P. pruinosus, the association between the rate of a random ramble and the presence of microvibrations was statistically significant (F = 5.01; p = 0.026). The average rate of a random ramble during the presence of vibrations was 1.86 and during their absence was 1.67. Isopods made more systematic turn alternation with the absence of vibrations. There was no significant effect of vibrations on the length of the time spent in the labyrinth (F = 0.03; p = 0.862). The average time spent in the labyrinth with the presence of vibrations was 37 sec while with the absence of vibrations it was 38 sec. There was no significant association between the presence of vibrations and change of turn alternation (F = 2.67; p = 0.103). An average number of changes in turn alternation with the presence of vibrations was 1.13 and 0.74 when vibrations were absent.
In the case of P. scaber, we found out the significant associations between increased temperature and the rate of a random ramble (F = 21.84; p < 0.001). The average rate of random ramble during exposure to increased temperatures was 1.92, while at lower temperatures it was 1.52. Thus, this species made less alternating turns in an increased temperature environment. Results also showed a statistically significant association between the time spent in the labyrinth and increased temperature (F = 30.65; p < 0.001). Individuals exposed to increased temperatures ran through the labyrinth with an average time of 58 seconds while in lower temperatures it was 25 seconds. Thus, isopods spent more time in a maze when temperatures were increased. We also found a significant association between temperatures and changes in turn alternation pattern (F = 25.56; p < 0.001). An average number of changes of turn alternation during exposure to increased temperature was 1.33 in comparison to 0.30 at lower temperature. An increasing number of changes in turn alternation was observed in woodlice behaviour when the temperature was increased.
For P. pruinosus, the associations between increased temperature and the rate of a random ramble were not significant (F = 0.02; p = 0.891). The average rate of random ramble during exposure to increased temperatures was 1.76, while at lower temperatures it was 1.77. There was no significant association between the time spent in the labyrinth and increased temperature (F = 0.79; p = 0.375) for this species. The average time spent in the labyrinth was 35 seconds per individuals exposed to increased temperatures and 40 seconds per those exposed to lower temperatures. We prove the existence of a significant association between temperatures and change of turn alternation pattern (F = 4.44; p = 0.036). An average number of changes in turn alternation during exposure to increased temperature was 1.2; for lower temperatures it was 0.7. For P. pruinosus, the increased temperature significantly increased the number of changes in turn alternation pattern.
Combination of increased temperatures and the presence of vibrations has significant effect on rate of random ramble (F = 8.99; p < 0.001; Fig.
For case of P. pruinosus, combination of increased temperatures together with presence of vibrations has no significant effect on rate of random ramble (F = 1.69; p = 0.170; Fig.
Characteristics of movement in T-maze of P. scaber (a, c, e) and P. pruinosus (b, d, f) at different treatments: a, b intensity of random/unspecific ramble, i.e., reversed value of systematic turn alternation c, d speed of passing through the labyrinth e, f number of self-corrective turns. Treatments V-/V+ mean absence/presence of substrate vibrations and T-/T+ mean low/high temperature.
We also found out a significant weak positive correlation (R = 0.32, p < 0.00001.) between the intensity of the turn alternation of P. scaber (i.e., the probability of running to one of the ends, indicating systematic turn alternation like A or D) and the speed of passage through the maze. When P. scaber ran slowly, there was higher probability that it will reach one of the “wrong” ends, which indicates unsystematic alternating turns. Results for P. pruinosus show no correlation (R = 0.06, p = 0.264619) between the intensity of the turn alternation and the speed rate of passage through a maze.
Group of two or more woodlice in contact were considered to be an aggregate. The distributions of the individuals were determined by counting the number of aggregated individuals in each box every 10 min during the 120-min experiment.
For P. scaber the results showed that there is a statistically significant difference in aggregation dynamics of isopods exposed to vibrations (F = 5.71; p = 0.003). Fig.
In P. pruinosus, vibrations together with increased temperature had a significant effect on the dynamic and size of aggregation (F = 83.52; p < 0.001). A higher number of aggregated individuals was observed among isopods exposed to lower temperatures combined with the presence of vibrations (after an hour, half of the total of 30 individuals were in aggregations). In comparison, numbers of aggregated isopods were the lowest at increased temperature with the absence of vibrations. In all of the observed variants it can be seen a slight increase in the number of aggregated individuals over time (Fig.
Our results showed that the effect of vibrations on the rate of turn alternation of P. scaber was not significant. When the substrate did not vibrate, P. pruinosus significantly increased turn alternations. For both species, the vibrations did not affect the time spent in the labyrinth or changed turn alternation pattern. This is probably because neither P. scaber nor P. pruinosus has any stridulatory or auditory organs to absorb vibrations. Those can be found in species like A. officinalis that is probably able to generate and receive vibrations (
A different effect of vibrations to turn alternations was reported by
An increased temperature did not stress P. scaber, because isopods spent more time in the maze and alternated their turns less systematically. This result is unexpected because it is contrary to the expectation based on the findings of
Vibrations, together with increased temperature, have a significant effect on the rate of random ramble, time spent in the labyrinth, as well as the change of turn alternation in P. scaber. These results have the same pattern as those with increased temperature alone. Apparently, vibrations were not stressful for P. scaber, probably due to its origin in the city environment.
Porcellio scaber showed a statistically significant difference in aggregation dynamics after their exposure to vibrations. At lower temperatures, somewhat stable aggregations of ~ 23–26 individuals were formed. At increased temperatures, within half an hour, the number of aggregated isopods increased to approximately 28 individuals and then stabilised. This is probably because the optimal temperature for P. scaber is 21 °C (
The aggregation of P. pruinosus was significantly affected by vibrations along with increased temperatures. Isopods aggregated more when exposed to lower temperatures. In an hour after the exposure more than half of the individuals were aggregated. This is in agreement with results of
Porcellionides pruinosus aggregated in greater numbers during the presence of vibrations than during non-vibration treatment. The same results showed an experiment by
Based on our findings, the pairing model species – stressor can be further refined for more significant results. Further research should be aimed at how stressful various temperature ranges for different isopod species are. Due to the ability of woodlice to acclimate to substrate vibrations, the future use of vibrations in experimental studies is very problematic. Perhaps shorter experiments with low air humidity as a stressor can be less difficult as our knowledge about the demands of different species is sufficient. The ability of different isopod species to habituate to stress factors could also play a certain role in this matter and future studies of this topic are encouraged.
Our results showed that for P. scaber and P. pruinosus, vibrations are not a stressful factor. This may be related to the fact that tested individuals have been collected in an urban environment where road and rail transport is a permanent source of substrate microvibrations, and the isopods are used to it. The increased temperature was a stressor only for P. scaber. This species did not show any major response to increased temperature; it went through the labyrinth more slowly at increased temperatures, and although they made more changes, they alternated turns less intensely. This behaviour could be caused by previous too long exposition to experimental conditions. Porcellionides pruinosus was not stressed by the increased temperature, which is probably caused by their occurrence in composts, where the temperature is often increased due to intense microbial decomposition. The aggregation dynamics of P. scaber was affected by the increased temperature. Initially, at increased temperatures isopods aggregated less or formed more unstable aggregations than the control group, but later the aggregations were stable and slightly larger than in the control group. Thus, the aggregation behaviour of P. scaber shows certain degree of stress, but its interpretation is relatively complicated. We were not able to confirm that P. pruinosus was stressed by vibrations or temperature, so it was not possible to make the comparison of the aggregation behaviour and the degree of stress. These results suggest that factors that have been used as stressors for specific species in some studies cannot be automatically used as stressors for other terrestrial isopod species.
This study was partly supported by an internal grant of the Faculty of Science of Palacky University Olomouc (IGA_PrF_2021_014). We are very grateful to Camila Wood and Giuseppe Montesanto for their helpful comments, advice, and valuable suggestions, which helped us highly improve our manuscript.