Research Article |
Corresponding author: Primož Zidar ( primoz.zidar@bf.uni-lj.si ) Academic editor: Ivanklin Campos-Filho
© 2022 Primož Zidar, Žiga Fišer.
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Citation:
Zidar P, Fišer Ž (2022) Avoidance behaviour toxicity tests should account for animal gregariousness: a case study on the terrestrial isopod Porcellio scaber. In: De Smedt P, Taiti S, Sfenthourakis S, Campos-Filho IS (Eds) Facets of terrestrial isopod biology. ZooKeys 1101: 87-108. https://doi.org/10.3897/zookeys.1101.76711
|
Avoidance behaviour enables woodlice to escape suboptimal environmental conditions and to mitigate harmful effects of pollutants. However, several studies have shown that at least in some woodlice species the tendency to aggregate can lead to suboptimal responses as the between-conspecific attraction can outweigh the aversive stimuli. The present study evaluated the influence of gregariousness on the behaviour of Porcellio scaber in a heterogeneously polluted environment. The hypothesis was that the tendency for aggregation outweighs the tendency for exploratory activity, therefore animals in groups will be less active. Consequently, this will affect their avoidance of polluted environmental patches. To test this hypothesis, isolated individuals or pairs of individuals were monitored in free-choice arenas where animals could select between uncontaminated and pyrethrin-contaminated soils. Animals were video recorded for 3 h in darkness using infrared light and analysed for avoidance behaviour and locomotor activity. In general, isolated animals were more locomotory active and avoided the contaminated soil more than paired animals. It can be concluded that aggregation behaviour suppresses exploratory behaviour and consequently also the avoidance of polluted environments. This should be accounted for when interpreting results of avoidance tests with groups of gregarious animals, which may underestimate the effect of pollutants.
Aggregation, ecotoxicology, locomotion, Isopoda, Oniscidea, woodlice
In the modern world not only heterogeneous distribution of resources but also environmental pollution is forcing animals to an exploratory behaviour. The shorter the time animals spend in a polluted environment, the more likely are they to survive. To do so, sensing, locomotor activity, spatial orientation, and appropriate storage of information about the new environment are crucial.
There is no doubt that terrestrial isopods can sense certain pollutants as many studies have shown their avoidance of soil or food polluted with metals (
Avoidance behaviour as an endpoint is frequently used in ecotoxicological studies to determine soil quality (
In this study we evaluated the influence of gregariousness on the behavioural response of individuals in a heterogeneously polluted environment. We hypothesized that the tendency for aggregation outweighs the tendency for exploratory activity, therefore animals in a group will be less active. Consequently, this will affect their avoidance of contaminated soil. To test this hypothesis, we monitored the locomotor activity and avoidance response of the terrestrial isopod Porcellio scaber Latreille, 1804 in a heterogeneously polluted environment.
Porcellio scaber is one of the most frequently used species in toxicity testing (
For the experiments, laboratory raised individuals were used. On the day of the recording, adult male specimens in the intermoult phase (
Isolated individuals or individuals accompanied by a conspecific were monitored in free-choice experiments where animals could select between uncontaminated and pyrethrin-contaminated soils. For this purpose, circular transparent polypropylene (PP) pots meant for food packaging (diameter 9.5 cm, height 6.0 cm) were used as test arenas. Arenas were divided into two equally sized chambers with a 3.5 cm high PP barrier (Fig.
Approximately 20 min prior recording a 3.0 g of Lufa 2.2 soil (Speyer, Germany) was added on top of the plaster that was previously saturated with tap water. Soil was previously dried, grinded, and sifted through a 0.5 mm sieve. Homogeneously granulated substrate prevented additional tactile stimuli that can affect animals’ activity (
For each new recording a new substrate was prepared, both plaster and soil, but pots were re-used after thorough washing with tap water. This prevented any other chemical stimuli besides insecticides to affect animals’ activity. For example, it is known that pheromones in woodlice faeces promote aggregation behaviour (
Pyrethrins were used as a soil contaminant in all experiments in this study. Pyrethrins are chemicals of natural origin with insecticidal action that have been used to control pests indoor and outdoor since the early 1800’s (
Soil was contaminated for 1 h prior the recording with the insecticide product Flora Kenyatox Verde Plus (Unichem, Slovenia) which contained 0.2% of pyrethrin. The insecticide product was well shaken and 50 µL, 100 µL, 150 µL, and 200 µL was added to 20 g of dry soil and mixed well with a spatula. The obtained concentrations were 2.5, 5.0, 7.5, and 10.0 mL of insecticide formulation per kg dry soil which roughly correspond to 5.2, 10.4, 15.7, and 20.8 mg of pyrethrin per kg dry soil.
One-hour prior video recording animals were marked dorsally across several tergites with partly dried enamel white paint to increase the visual contrast between woodlice and the substrate. In the case of two animals per arena only one animal was marked. It was reported previously that some external markers (e.g., nail polish) may affect diurnal activity and food consumption in some isopod species (
At the start of recordings one or two animals were placed in test arenas, always in chamber A containing uncontaminated soil. The animals were then continuously recorded for 3 h. In similar studies, animals’ activity was tracked for 2 or 4 hours (
To provide a recording environment isolated from the outside light and other unwanted perturbances, a specially designed recording box was used. The box measured 55, 100, and 100 cm in depth, width, and height, respectively. Animals were recorded only under infrared light (850 nm) to avoid any light-induced behaviour (
Videos were captured in VirtualDub 1.10.4 at 5 frames per second and a Full HD resolution (1920 × 1080 pixels). One pixel corresponded to 0.13 mm.
Videos were first analysed via video-tracking in Bonsai 2.4.0 (
Next, videos were examined also manually. In this way a sequence of several hundred frames when animals were at rest with antennae close to the body and no detectable movements was selected. Based on allocations of the centroid between two consecutive frames in this sequence the upper limit for noise was determined (0.2 pixels or 0.022 mm per frame). The upper limit for non-locomotor activity (0.8 pixels or 0.088 mm per frame) was determined based on a sequence of several hundred frames when animals were feeding or digging. All larger allocations of the centroid between two consecutive frames (> 0.8 pixels) were considered as locomotor activity. Additionally, the number of visits to contaminated soil (chamber B) was counted.
Finally, raw trajectories were imported to MS Excel and used to calculate behavioural variables: the proportion of time that isopods spent on the uncontaminated soil (chamber A), the overall duration of locomotor and non-locomotor activity. Change in animal’s position during locomotor activity was calculated as path length. Average speed was calculated as the total path length divided by the total time of locomotor activity.
All statistical analyses were performed in R 4.1.1. (R Core Team), except for the probit regression and Pearson’s correlation which were performed in SPSS 27.0. All plots were drawn using the latter software as well.
Sample sizes were relatively small, data were often non-normally distributed, and between-group variance was often heteroscedastic, therefore robust statistical methods implemented in the R package WRS2 were employed (
The avoidance response was determined in four different ways. In the first approach, we assumed that avoidance of contaminated soil was successful if animals spent more than half of the recording time on uncontaminated soil. Therefore, the percentage of time on uncontaminated soil was tested against a fixed value of 50% for each concentration and the control. To do so a robust one-sample test was applied using the function onesampb(), the M-estimator of central tendency, and 10,000 bootstraps (to estimate the 95% confidence intervals).
In the second approach, data on the time spent on uncontaminated soil were used to estimate the median effective concentration of pyrethrin (EC50) for avoidance response. Data were first transformed by a formula adapted from
In the third approach, the avoidance response was assessed by the number of visits to contaminated soil. A robust two-way ANOVA was applied using the number of visits to contaminated soil as a dependent variable, while concentration treatment (0, 2.5, 5.0, 7.5, 10.0) and number of animals (isolated, paired) were used as independent categorical variables whose interaction was tested as well. For two-way ANOVA the function pbad2way(), the M-estimator of central tendency, and 5,000 bootstraps were used. Next, post-hoc comparisons were performed to find at which concentrations the response differed from the control in isolated (4 tests) and paired (4 tests) animals, as well as to find at which concentration treatments the response of isolated and paired animals was mutually different (5 tests). For this, robust independent two-sample tests were applied using the function pb2gen(), the M-estimator of central tendency, and 10,000 bootstraps (to estimate the 95% confidence intervals). P-values were adjusted via the method of
In the fourth approach to estimate avoidance response, the location of animals at the end of the 3-h recordings was used. The percentage of animals on the uncontaminated soil was calculated.
Behavioural variables on isopod activity (duration, path length, average speed) were analysed as described for the third approach to avoidance response estimation. First, a robust two-way ANOVA was performed with the specific behavioural variable as a dependent variable, while concentration treatment and number of animals were used as independent categorical variables whose interaction was also tested. Upon significant effects, the same post-hoc comparisons procedure as stated above was applied. Additionally, Pearson’s correlation between the duration of locomotor activity and path length was calculated.
Data on average speed at contaminated (chamber B) and uncontaminated (chamber A) soil involved two measurements per individual. To account for repeated measurements, a robust two-way between-within subjects ANOVA was applied. Separate models were fitted for isolated and paired animals. In both, average speed was used as a dependent variable, while concentration treatment and arena chamber (A – uncontaminated, B – contaminated) were used as independent categorical variables whose interaction was also tested. In this case the function bwtrim() and the 20% trimmed mean as an estimate of central tendency were used. Next, post-hoc comparisons were performed to find at which concentration treatments average speed differed between the two arena chambers in isolated (5 tests) and paired (5 tests) animals. For this, robust dependent two-sample tests were applied using the function yuend() and the 20% trimmed mean as an estimate of central tendency. P-values were adjusted according to
Isolated animals of the control group spent, on average, the same amount of time in both chambers containing uncontaminated soil (Suppl. material
Isolated animals showed avoidance behaviour to 5.0, 7.5, and 10.0 mL of pyrethrin formulation per kg dry soil as they spent significantly more time on uncontaminated soil (Suppl. material
The percentage of time that Porcellio scaber spent on uncontaminated soil (chamber A) within the 3 h of observation. In a free-choice experiment isolated or paired animal could select between soil contaminated with pyrethrin and uncontaminated soil. Key: box: 25th, 50th, and 75th percentile; whiskers: value ≤ 1.5 IQR; o – outlier: 3 IQR ≤ value > 1.5 IQR; + – extreme: value > 3 IQR; * – significantly different than 50%, p < 0.05; ** – as previous, but p < 0.01.
The estimated EC50 for isolated animals was 2.8 mL of pyrethrin formulation per kg dry soil (95% confidence interval: 2.7–5.0 mL/kg) while EC50 for paired animals was 7.9 mL of pyrethrin formulation per kg dry soil (95% CI: 5.1–21.4 mL/kg), much higher than for isolated animals.
The ANOVA showed that the number of visits to contaminated soil differed significantly between concentration treatments (p = 0.01) and number of animals (p = 0.006). However, we found no interaction effect between these two variables (p = 0.189), meaning that the difference between isolated and paired animals did not differ among concentration treatments.
Post-hoc comparisons between different pyrethrin concentrations and the control revealed that in both isolated and paired animals the number of visits to contaminated soil significantly decreased at concentrations of 5.0 mL of pyrethrin formulation per kg dry soil or higher (Suppl. material
The number of visits to contaminated soil (chamber B) that Porcellio scaber made during the 3 h of observation. In a free-choice experiment, isolated or paired animals could select between soil contaminated with pyrethrin and uncontaminated soil. Key: box: 25th, 50th, and 75th percentile; whiskers: value ≤ 1.5 IQR; o – outlier: 3 IQR ≤ value > 1.5 IQR; + – extreme: value > 3 IQR; ↓ – significantly lower than control, p < 0.05; * – significant difference between isolated and paired animals, p < 0.05; ** – as previous, but p < 0.01.
After 3 h, six isolated and five paired animals of control groups were found in chamber A, and the rest in chamber B. The location of pyrethrin exposed animals did not correspond to the concentrations of pyrethrin in soil (Table
The percentage of animals (Porcellio scaber) located on uncontaminated soil (chamber A) at the end of 3 h observation. In a free-choice experiment, animals could select between soil contaminated with pyrethrin and uncontaminated soil.
Pyrethrin concentration (mL/kg dry soil) | Isolated animals (%) | Paired animals (%) |
---|---|---|
0 | 50 | 42 |
2.5 | 50 | 62.5 |
5.0 | 92 | 25 |
7.5 | 75 | 87.5 |
10.0 | 50 | 75 |
In general, the animals were locomotory active from several minutes up to 1 h (Fig.
Post-hoc comparisons between different pyrethrin concentrations and the control revealed that in isolated animals, locomotor activity decreased when exposed to pyrethrin formulation in soil (Suppl. material
Duration of locomotor activity of Porcellio scaber within the 3 h of observation. In a free-choice experiment, isolated or paired animals could select between soil contaminated with pyrethrin and uncontaminated soil. Key: box: 25th, 50th, and 75th percentile; whiskers: value ≤ 1.5 IQR; o – outlier: 3 IQR ≤ value > 1.5 IQR; + – extreme: value > 3 IQR; ↓ – significantly lower than control, p < 0.05; * – significant difference between isolated and paired animals, p < 0.05.
The length of the path that the animals walked during the observation correlated with the duration of locomotor activity (Pearson correlation: r = 0.893, p < 0.001) and varied from 0.8 m up to nearly 37 m (Fig.
Post-hoc comparisons between different pyrethrin concentrations and the control, revealed that in isolated animals the path length decreased with increasing concentration of pyrethrin formulation in soil, but was significantly lower than the control only at concentrations of 5.0 and 7.5 mL/kg dry soil (Suppl. material
The average speed of locomotion of control animals was 1.3–4.6 mm/s in isolated animals and 2.2–4.2 mm/s in paired animals (Fig.
Post-hoc comparisons between different pyrethrin concentrations and the control revealed that in isolated animals the average speed significantly increased at 2.5 and 5.0 ml/kg dry soil (Suppl. material
Overall length of the path that Porcellio scaber walked within the 3 h of observation. In a free-choice experiment, isolated or paired animals could select between soil contaminated with pyrethrin and uncontaminated soil. Key: box: 25th, 50th, and 75th percentile; whiskers: value ≤ 1.5 IQR; o – outlier: 3 IQR ≤ value > 1.5 IQR; + – extreme: value > 3 IQR; ↓ – significantly lower than control, p < 0.05; * – significant difference between isolated and paired animals, p < 0.05.
Average speed of locomotion of Porcellio scaber within the 3 h of observation. In a free-choice experiment, isolated or paired animals could select between soil contaminated with pyrethrin and uncontaminated soil. Key: box: 25th, 50th, and 75th percentile; whiskers: value ≤ 1.5 IQR; o – outlier: 3 IQR ≤ value > 1.5 IQR; + – extreme: value > 3 IQR; ↑ – significantly higher than control, p < 0.05.
Further analyses focused on average speed of isolated and paired animals in the two arena chambers (uncontaminated vs. contaminated) at different pyrethrin concentrations. The ANOVA for isolated animals showed that their average speed depended on the arena chamber (p = 0.001) and concentration (p = 0.015), but the interaction of these two variables had no significant effect on the response (p = 0.268). The latter meaning that the difference in average speed between uncontaminated and contaminated soil did not differ among concentration treatments. Post-hoc comparisons revealed that isolated animals moved significantly faster on contaminated soil than on uncontaminated soil at 2.5 and 10.0 mL of pyrethrin formulation per kg dry soil (Suppl. material
The ANOVA for paired animals showed that their average speed depended on the arena chamber (p = 0.003) and concentration (p = 0.007), as well as the interaction of these two variables (p = 0.045). The significant interaction effect reveals that the difference in average speed between uncontaminated and contaminated soil differed among concentration treatments. Post-hoc comparisons revealed that paired animals did not move significantly faster on contaminated soil compared to uncontaminated soil at any concentration treatment, although marginally significant differences and large effect sizes in this direction were observed at concentrations 2.5 and 10.0 mL/kg dry soil (Suppl. material
Average speed of locomotion of Porcellio scaber on uncontaminated soil (chamber A) and soil contaminated with pyrethrin (chamber B) for isolated animals (A) and paired animals (B). In a free-choice experiment, isolated or paired animals could select between soil contaminated with pyrethrin and uncontaminated soil. In plot A, two extreme values for chamber B are not shown: at conc. 2.5 (value = 12.99) and conc. 10 (value = 25.77). Key: box: 25th, 50th, and 75th percentile; whiskers: value ≤ 1.5 IQR; o – outlier: 3 IQR ≤ value > 1.5 IQR; + – extreme: value > 3 IQR; * – significant difference between uncontaminated and contaminated soil, p < 0.05.
Non-locomotor activity of animals lasted from 8 to 49 min (Fig.
Post-hoc comparisons between different pyrethrin concentrations and the control revealed that in isolated animals, non-locomotor activity significantly decreased at all pyrethrin concentrations used (Suppl. material
Duration of non-locomotor activity of Porcellio scaber within the 3 h of observation. In a free-choice experiment, isolated or paired animals could select between soil contaminated with pyrethrin and uncontaminated soil. Key: box: 25th, 50th, and 75th percentile; whiskers: value ≤ 1.5 IQR; o – outlier: 3 IQR ≤ value > 1.5 IQR; + – extreme: value > 3 IQR; ↓ – significantly lower than control, p < 0.01; *** – significant difference between isolated and paired animals, p < 0.001.
We investigated the influence of aggregation on avoidance behaviour and activity of Porcellio scaber exposed to contaminated soil. Individual animals (isolated) or animals in pairs were recorded for 3 h in two chambered arenas where they could select between uncontaminated soil and soil contaminated with the insecticide pyrethrin. Time spent on uncontaminated soil revealed more successful avoidance of contaminated soil in isolated than in paired animals. This measure of avoidance response was more sensitive and robust than the number of visits to contaminated soil or location of animals after a specific time since exposure. Animals unexposed to contaminated soil were significantly more active when isolated than when in pairs. This was evident from the duration of locomotory and non-locomotory activity. However, when exposed to pyrethrin the differences between isolated and paired animals decreased, although some differences in path length and average speed still indicated higher activity of isolated animals.
Although all animals started the experiment in chamber A, the control animals showed that the initial position of animals in the arena does not affect the time the control animals spent in each chamber of the arena (A or B, both containing uncontaminated soil). In published avoidance behaviour test protocols, animals were introduced into the test arenas differently: in the middle between control and test soils (
During 3 h of exposure isolated isopods clearly avoided soil with pyrethrin formulation already at a concentration of 5.0 mL of formulation per kg dry soil, which roughly corresponds to 10.4 mg of pyrethrin per kg dry soil. This concentration is the lowest observed effective concentration (LOEC) in this study. According to our previous study (
The EC50 value obtained in this study for isolated animals (2.8 mL of formulation or 5.9 mg of pyrethrin per kg dry soil) was almost twice lower than the EC50 value obtained in the 48-h avoidance test with animals in groups (9.7 mg of pyrethrin per kg dry soil;
The frequency at which isolated animals visited contaminated soil decreased with the increased concentration of pyrethrin in soil and was in concordance with the time that animals spent on the contaminated soil. In paired animals the frequency of visits to contaminated soil was generally lower than that of isolated animals but their retention time on contaminated soil was much higher. As reported by
The location of animals (on uncontaminated vs. contaminated soil) at a given time also does not necessarily reflect the avoidance response as has been reported previously (Odendaal and Reinecke 1997;
Our results show that the accepted standard toxicity tests relying on avoidance behaviour of a group of individuals as an endpoint should probably be reconsidered when performed with gregarious animals like isopods that exhibit strong aggregation behaviour. These tests tend to underestimate the effect of the toxicant. The reason for this is twofold and originates from the dynamic hierarchy of the two independent stimuli of an opposite sign provided by the toxicant (negative) and the presence of conspecifics (positive) against the background environment (neutral). When isopods are introduced to a novel environment such as the test arena, they first explore it in approximately random movement. Note that the location of the negative stimuli is fixed while the positive stimuli move(s) randomly within the arena. When both stimuli appear on different halves of the arena the choice is clear. The dilemma arises when both negative and positive stimuli occur at the same arena half. In such scenarios, the animals’ response will depend on the relative strength of both stimuli. When toxicant concentration is high, it will prevail over conspecific attraction and animals will eventually aggregate at the optimal arena half. However, when toxicant concentration is low, conspecific attraction prevails over the negative effects of toxicant and animals will aggregate at the non-optimal arena half. The observed result is a lack of avoidance response interpreted as no effect of toxicant. However, when individual animals, and not groups, are tested at the same low toxicant concentration, they show clear avoidance signalling harmful effects. Thus, aggregation can mask the real effect of the toxicant and tests with groups of animals will tend to overestimate the effective toxicant concentration.
The real concern is that in natural populations animals will practically always be in a group. Groups of aggregating animals avoid high toxicant concentrations but are much less effective at avoiding low and moderate concentrations although harmful. Consequently, in a heterogeneously polluted environment these concentrations might eventually cause more damage as animals will be exposed to them longer and accumulate their negative effects, while they will retreat from higher concentrations. The standard toxicity tests with avoidance behaviour will however fail to reveal this. Thus, for gregarious animals we should rather estimate the effective concentration for both individual animals and those in a group. Although counterintuitive at first sight, the range of concentrations between these two values may be effectively most harmful to the natural populations.
Finally, group size and social composition of its members are additional factors that for sure add to the variation of toxicity tests results and should be considered in model species for which this is relevant and possible. Further investigation is needed in this direction. From the broadest perspective, aggregation of gregarious animals will likely affect the outcome and interpretation of any kind of choice tests with any kind of aversive (e.g., light, predator pheromones) or favourable (e.g., humidity, thigmotactic shelters) stimuli.
Locomotor behaviour of terrestrial isopods was recognised as a sensitive biomarker of exposure to different pollutants (Bayley et al. 1995,
We conclude that:
This research was supported by the Slovenian Research Agency through the Research Core Funding P1-0184.
Table S1–S7
Data type: Statistical data
Explanation note: Avoidance behavior toxicity tests should account for animal gregariousness: a case study on isopod Porcellio scaber.