Research Article |
Corresponding author: Lauren VanMaurik ( lvanmaurik@mail.usf.edu ) Academic editor: Ingo S. Wehrtmann
© 2014 Lauren VanMaurik, Jennifer Wortham.
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Citation:
VanMaurik LN, Wortham JL (2014) Grooming as a secondary behavior in the shrimp Macrobrachium rosenbergii (Crustacea, Decapoda, Caridea). In: Wehrtmann IS, Bauer RT (Eds) Proceedings of the Summer Meeting of the Crustacean Society and the Latin American Association of Carcinology, Costa Rica, July 2013. ZooKeys 457: 55-77. https://doi.org/10.3897/zookeys.457.6292
|
The giant freshwater prawn, Macrobrachium rosenbergii, is a large shrimp extensively used in aquaculture whose grooming behaviors were analyzed in this study. Macrobrachium rosenbergii exhibits three unique male morphotypes that differ in their behavior, morphology and physiology: small-clawed males (SM), orange-clawed males (OC) and blue-clawed males (BC). The largest and most dominant males, BC males, are predicted to have significantly different grooming behaviors compared to females and the other two male morphotypes. These BC males may be too large and bulky to efficiently groom and may dedicate more time to mating and agonistic interactions than grooming behaviors. Observations were conducted to look at the prevalence of grooming behaviors in the absence and presence of conspecifics and to determine if any differences in grooming behavior exist among the sexes and male morphotypes. Significant differences in the grooming behaviors of all individuals (females and male morphotypes) were found. BC males tended to have the highest grooming time budget (percent of time spent grooming) while SM males had a relatively low grooming time budget. The grooming behaviors of the male morphotypes differed, indicating while these males play distinct, separate roles in the social hierarchy, they also have different grooming priorities. The conditions in which M. rosenbergii are cultured may result in increased body fouling, which may vary, depending on the grooming efficiencies and priorities of these male morphotypes. Overall, grooming behaviors were found to be a secondary behavior which only occurred when primary behaviors such as mating, feeding or fighting were not present.
Grooming, aquaculture, Macrobrachium rosenbergii
A behavioral hierarchy occurs among certain behaviors which are deemed essential to an organism. Ranking of behaviors by individuals is necessary when an organism is in conflict situations such as foraging (
The environment that an organism inhabits is also an important factor in the behavioral decision making process. Organisms in resource-limiting environments or social situations (i.e. competition, mating, agonistic interactions) should prioritize those behaviors with the greatest resource profitability (primary behaviors, i.e. searching for food or mates) before other subordinate behaviors (
Grooming is a behavior for removing fouling debris and organisms from body surfaces (
Autogrooming in crustaceans is important for removing macro- and microscopic fouling organisms, debris, sedimentation, and algae from body surfaces (
Grooming behaviors have been studied for many crustacean groups, especially in decapod crustaceans such as penaeid and caridean shrimps (
The genus Macrobrachium has over 240 species (
Agonistic behaviors and social structure of Macrobrachium rosenbergii have been extensively studied due to its use in aquaculture (
The three male morphotypes of M. rosenbergii exhibit behavioral differences based on their position within the social hierarchy (
Due to the importance of Macrobrachium rosenbergii in aquaculture, understanding the grooming behaviors of this species is vital for implementing ways to increase yield and growth by decreasing the potential fouling affecting the morphotypes. The most profitable size is the large BC males and development into this terminal male morphotype depends upon surviving through the SM and OC male phases. We hypothesized M. rosenbergii will: (1) have similar grooming behaviors to other caridean shrimps; (2) BC males will have less time available for grooming than other males due to time dedicated to the protection of females, defense of territories and dominance behaviors; and (3) the grooming will be a secondary behavior as hypothesized by
Macrobrachium rosenbergii were transported overnight from Texas in April 2012 to the University of Tampa. Individual shrimps were added to labeled plastic holding containers (5.5-L), with pre-drilled holes that allowed water flow, and then placed in an 1816-liter fiberglass aquaculture tank with filtered, continuous flowing, aerated water. The individual containers reduced physical contact and agonistic interactions, ensuring that both shrimps’ appendages remained intact and death by cannibalism was eliminated. The containers allowed visual and pheromonal contact among individuals as water was able to flow through the pre-drilled holes. The three male morphotypes (SM, OC and BC males) were distinguished by morphological characteristics and correlations among mass and body measurements (
The grooming data were analyzed to determine if they met the criteria for parametric statistics. If normality assumptions were not met, then non-parametric statistics were used. Along with variability in individual behavior and failure to meet normality, the grooming data were analyzed using non-parametric statistics. Non-parametric statistical tests used included the Kruskal-Wallis test and the Mann-Whitney U test. Regression analyses were also used. Statistical significance was determined by p-value of less than 0.05.
Behavioral observations were conducted to study the grooming behaviors of Macrobrachium rosenbergii. The null hypotheses of no difference in relative time budgets allocated to grooming behaviors among the three male morphotypes and between sexes were tested. Individuals were tested in isolation, which helped reduce primary behaviors such as fighting and mating. Each shrimp was used once (N=94) in these solitary observations and placed into a 19-L (40 cm × 25 cm × 20 cm) aquarium with black backing and natural rocky substratum. The black backing ensured that the shrimp would not be influenced by either surrounding shrimps in other tanks or the observer. Water in the aquarium was continually filtered and frequently replaced with water from the aquaculture tanks. Shrimps were allowed to acclimate for 24-hr before testing and were not used if they had molted within seven days. Females with embryos (“eggs”) on their pleopods were not observed to control for the behavior among males and females. All grooming behaviors were recorded during the daylight cycle for 30-min using a digital recording device and then later transcribed to data sheets following the methods of
The null hypothesis that all behaviors will be equally prioritized was tested. To observe how social interactions with conspecifics affect grooming behaviors, each male morphotype (SM, OC, and BC males) and females were placed in a grooming situation where shrimps could physically touch through antennular and cheliped contact (but not fight) via holes in the individual containers. The objective was to compare grooming behaviors of individuals in an environment without visual or minor physical cues (Solitary Grooming – Observations #1) to that of an environment with visual and physical cues (Social Grooming – Observations #2). These latter observations differed from the Solitary Grooming (Observations #1) because individuals in the isolated situation only had chemical contact through water with other shrimps but did not have visual or minor physical input that was present in the Social Grooming (Observations #2). Visual and minor physical contact with conspecifics was expected to reduce frequency and time allocated to grooming behaviors in these observations, since these behaviors have been predicted to be secondary. Shrimps (N=8; two shrimps of male morphotypes plus females) were observed in the aquaculture tanks in their individual containers for 15-min and their grooming behaviors were recorded. These shrimps were randomly selected from the first observations (Solitary Grooming) and observed 24-hr after being used in the first observations. The same process of recording behaviors was used as in the Solitary Grooming (Observations #1). The data collected in these social observations were extrapolated (multiplied by 2) in order to compare the data to those from the Solitary Grooming (Observations #1) (15-min × 2 = 30-min).
The null hypothesis that all behaviors are equally prioritized was tested to determine how agonistic interactions (primary behaviors) affect the priority of grooming behaviors of the male morphotypes (BC, OC, and SM) and females. The objective of these observations was to compare the frequency of grooming behaviors in an environment without visual cues (Solitary Grooming, Observations #1) to that of an environment with physical contact (Agonistic Interactions, Observations #3). If grooming behavior is a secondary action incurring the same energy cost as primary behavior (i.e. mating, fighting, displaying), grooming behaviors should be reduced in time and frequency during these observations compared to both the solitary and social observations (Observations #1 and #2, respectively). During these observations shrimps had physical contact with another individual in a test arena and their grooming behaviors were recorded along with all other behaviors such as swimming, antennular touching, mating, fighting and non-agonistic interactions (interactions with no aggressive behaviors). This is different from the Social Grooming (Observations #2) due to the increased level of interaction (i.e. fighting, mating possible). In Observations #3, shrimps were paired based on morphotype and sex for a total of ten different treatments; there were two trials of each treatment for a total of N=20 observations (Table
Experimental design of Agonistic Interactions (Observations #3), listing the ten treatments and the number of trials for each treatment. BC = blue-clawed males; OC = orange-clawed males; SM = small-clawed males; F = females.
Treatment | Individual #1 | Individual #2 | Sample Size |
---|---|---|---|
Treatment 1 | BC | BC | 2 |
Treatment 2 | BC | OC | 2 |
Treatment 3 | BC | SM | 2 |
Treatment 4 | BC | F | 2 |
Treatment 5 | OC | OC | 2 |
Treatment 6 | OC | SM | 2 |
Treatment 7 | OC | F | 2 |
Treatment 8 | SM | SM | 2 |
Treatment 9 | SM | F | 2 |
Treatment 10 | F | F | 2 |
Four appendages were observed actively grooming the body: third maxilliped (M3), first pereopod (P1), second pereopod (P2), and fifth pereopod (P5) (Fig.
Overall, the most frequently used grooming appendages for all individuals (females and the male morphotypes) were the M3 and P1, but there were no significant differences in the frequency of use among the grooming appendages among all three male morphotypes and females. The M3 and P1 were used equally among all groups (Kruskal-Wallis, H=3.55, P<0.001; Fig.
Grooming of the body parts in terms of frequency were analyzed. The total grooming actions in terms of frequency for all observations (N=94) was 2,838 behaviors. Common grooming behaviors observed include M3 scraping the sensory structures (A1 and A2), P1 brushing the respiratory structures (enclosed gills) and general body grooming by the pereiopods. There was no difference in use of a single appendage between the females and male morphotypes. The most frequently groomed part of the body was the first pereopods (P1) (which are also frequently used grooming appendages) (Fig.
Important sensory, locomotive, and morphological areas of the body were selected among the females and male morphotypes to determine if these areas were groomed equally. These areas and functionality include (1) the walking legs (P5–P8) (locomotive), (2) antennal scale (precision in agonistic interactions and steering and braking function) and (3) pleopods (reproduction and forward swimming). There were no significant differences in the grooming frequency of these selected areas for the females and all male morphotypes (i.e. frequency of grooming antennal scale of females and BC males were equal) (Mann-Whitney U, z=-1.41–0.77, P=0.16–0.97; Fig.
Mean frequency of body parts groomed of Macrobrachium rosenbergii females (N=21) and male morphotypes (SM: N=28, OC: N=25, BC: N=20) in a 30-min time period (mean ± SE). Body parts in graph labeled from anterior to posterior, left to right. Note: BC = blue-clawed males; F = females, OC = orange-clawed males; SM = small males.
Mean total frequency of grooming behaviors of Macrobrachium rosenbergii females (N=21) and male morphotypes (SM: N=28, OC: N=25, BC: N=20) in 30-min time period (mean ± SE). Note: BC = blue-clawed males; F = females, OC = orange-clawed males; SM = small males. No significant differences between the groups, p>0.05.
Although a part may be frequently groomed, it may not be groomed for a long amount of time. The amount of time spent grooming body parts was analyzed. In all 94 observations, the total time spent observing individuals was 47 hrs. Of those 47 hours, the total time spent grooming by all 94 shrimps was 35,132 sec (9.76 hrs). The part groomed for the longest average time was the gills (Fig.
When looking at the time spent grooming different body parts in the morphotypes, there were clear differences. There were significant differences in the time spent grooming the second pereopods (P2) between the BC males and all other groups (OC and SM males as well as F (Kruskal-Wallis, H=8.72, P=0.033; Mann-Whitney U, z=-2.73 to -2.02, P=0.006–0.044; Fig.
Mean time (sec) of body parts groomed of Macrobrachium rosenbergii females (N=21) and male morphotypes (SM: N=28, OC: N=25, BC: N=20) in a 30-min time period (mean ± SE). Body parts in graph labeled from anterior to posterior, left to right. Note: BC = blue-clawed males; F = females, OC = orange-clawed males; SM = small males. Different letters indicate significant differences among body parts (B is referring to the females and SM and OC males).
Mean total time (sec) spent grooming of Macrobrachium rosenbergii females (N=21) and male morphotypes (SM: N=28, OC: N=25, BC: N=20) in 30-min time period (mean ± SE). Note: BC = blue-clawed males; F = females, OC = orange-clawed males; SM = small males. No significant differences between the groups, p>0.05.
Overall in Macrobrachium rosenbergii, a generous proportion of time is spent grooming the body. The average time budget for grooming was 19.3%, indicating up to one-fifth of Macrobrachium rosenbergii’s time may be dedicated to grooming when primary behaviors (fighting, mating, etc.) are not present.
Of the females and male morphotypes, the BC males had the highest average time budget for grooming, 35.2%, which was significantly higher than that of females (10.2%) (Mann-Whitney U, z=-2.93, P=0.0033; Fig.
Mean time budget for grooming of Macrobrachium rosenbergii morphotypes (F: N=21, SM: N=28, OC: N=25, BC: N=20) in 30-min time period. Overall mean time budget of species is 19.3%. Note: BC = blue-clawed males; F = females, OC = orange-clawed males; SM = small males. Different letters indicate significant differences among groups.
In all social observations (N=8), not one grooming behavior occurred. Hence, the observations were ended prematurely at a lower sample size compared to the other observations (Observations #1 and #3). The shrimps were in a social situation where many behaviors such as searching for mates, displaying, touching and grooming can occur. The grooming time budget was 0% for all observations.
The paired shrimps (N=20; Table
The behavior that occurred for the longest time was non-agonistic interactions and grooming occurred for the shortest amount of time (Fig.
Overall, Macrobrachium rosenbergii showed similar grooming behaviors compared to other caridean shrimps including the usage of specific grooming appendages (third maxillipeds, first, second and fifth pereopods) (
Species | Grooming Time Budget (%) | Presence of Conspecifics? | Reference |
---|---|---|---|
Heptacarpus pictus | 27% | No |
|
Macrobrachium grandimanus | 25% | No |
|
Macrobrachium rosenbergii | 19% | No | Current study (Observations #1) |
Macrobrachium rosenbergii | 0% | Partial | Current study (Observations #2) |
Macrobrachium rosenbergii | 0.31% | Yes | Current study (Observations #3) |
Although there were similarities in the grooming behaviors of Macrobrachium rosenbergii and other carideans, the male morphotypes of M. rosenbergii differed in grooming behaviors. These morphotypes are known to differ in their behavior based on their niche in the social hierarchy, and therefore it is plausible their grooming behaviors and time dedicated to grooming may differ (
The SM and BC male morphotypes differ in their behavior yet they have the highest total frequency and time of grooming actions along with the highest time budgets for grooming. This may be attributed to the relative efficiencies of their grooming activities: SM males are highly mobile and may experience greater fouling pressures (
Although BC males have the highest grooming time budget, it appears that most of this time is spent in the grooming of the P2 appendage. The BC males groomed the P2 appendage frequently and for a long time, which may be due to the setal patch located on the propodus. The setal patch may participate in displays to ward off other males from territories (Correa et al. 2000). The P2 appendage is also used in the protection and defense of females. The fact that the BC males dedicate much time to the grooming of this appendage indicates it is may be important in maintaining the dominance position of these males in the social hierarchy.
We found that primary (higher priority) behaviors such as feeding and defense are of greater importance and should occur more frequently than grooming when primary behaviors are possible. Grooming was absent or rare when primary actions occurred (social grooming observations, Observations #2), therefore grooming should be considered as a secondary behavior, as hypothesized by
As hypothesized, Macrobrachium rosenbergii showed similar grooming behaviors and grooming time budgets compared to other caridean shrimps indicating grooming behaviors have evolved in response to the fouling pressures experienced in an aquatic environment. We found that the BC males dedicate much time to the care of the P2 appendages which are used in displays, protection and defense. This behavior contributes to the high time budget for grooming in the BC males. Grooming was also hypothesized to be a secondary behavior, only occurring when other behaviors are not essential (
Macrobrachium rosenbergii is frequently grown in aquaculture (
The authors wish to thank Drs. Thomas Crisman, Jason Rohr and Mark Rains for their support and comments of this work. The authors also wish to thank Mr. Rob Haughey and Mrs. Danielle Neveu for their laboratory assistance. Great thanks to Mr. Craig Upstrom at Aquaculture of Texas, Inc. for aiding in the supply of research shrimps. We thank Drs. Raymond Bauer, Martin Thiel and Abraham Miller for comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. This work was funded by the Department of Integrative Biology at the University of South Florida and the University of Tampa College of Natural and Health Sciences.